Is an Instructional Intervention Worthwhile?
The paper “How to Assess Whether an Instructional Intervention Has an Effect on Learning” by Richard E. Mayer critiques contemporary trends in educational psychology research and advocates for the primacy of experimental methodologies in assessing the effectiveness of instructional interventions. The paper centers on the increasing reliance on non-experimental studies in educational psychology journals and the implications this shift has for establishing causal relationships and providing actionable recommendations. Lin Zhang, Bill Cobern, John Sweller and I wrote an article[1] related to this problem recently: There is an Evidence Crisis in Science Educational Policy. It’s open access, do enjoy it!
Mayer begins by acknowledging the diversity of research methods—ranging from experimental to correlational and qualitative approaches—employed in educational psychology. However, he underscores the principle that research methods must align with research goals. For inquiries into the causal effects of interventions on learning outcomes, experimental methods, particularly randomised controlled trials, are the gold standard. These methods enable researchers to isolate the effects of an intervention by ensuring control over confounding variables and maintaining random assignment, thus providing the most robust evidence for causality.
In contrast, Mayer critiques the trend noted by Brady et al. (2023)[2], wherein non-experimental studies, such as correlational analyses and observational research, are increasingly used to draw causal inferences. While these methods are valuable for exploratory research and generating hypotheses, they fall short of the rigor needed to substantiate causal claims. This misalignment, Mayer argues, not only risks misleading conclusions but also undermines the practical recommendations made to educators and policymakers.
To illustrate his point, Mayer evaluates six empirical approaches to assess instructional interventions: experimental comparisons, modelling studies, cross-sectional comparisons, correlational studies, pre-post comparisons, and observational studies. Among these, experimental comparisons stand out for their strength in testing causal claims. Methods like correlational and observational studies, while useful in exploratory phases, are insufficient for making definitive statements about causality.
Mayer also acknowledges the nuanced perspectives of other scholars, such as Grosz and Zitzmann et al. (2023)[3], who recognize the utility of non-experimental methods in certain contexts but reiterate the unparalleled value of experiments for causal inference. These scholars also emphasize the need for better communication of research findings to stakeholders, ensuring that conclusions are appropriately contextualized and actionable.
In conclusion, Mayer calls for a recommitment to experimental research in educational psychology. While he acknowledges the role of diverse methodologies in the research ecosystem, he warns against the diminishing presence of experimental studies in the field’s leading journals. Without a strong foundation of experimental evidence, the field risks losing its capacity to make scientifically grounded recommendations that effectively guide educational practice and policy.
[1] Zhang, L., Kirschner, P.A., Cobern, W.W. et al. There is an evidence crisis in science educational policy. Educional Psychology Review, 34, 1157–1176 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-021-09646-1
[2] Brady, A. C., Griffin, M. M., Lewis, A. R., Fong, C. J., & Robinson, D. H. (2023). How scientific is educational psychology research? The increasing trend of squeezing causality and recommendations from non-intervention studies. Educational Psychology Review, 35, 37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-023-09759-9
[3] Grosz, M. (2023). Should researchers make causal inferences and recommendations for practice on the basis of non-experimental studies? Educational Psychology Review, 35, 37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-023-09777-7